Question

Design an ad for a new product using the elaboration likelihood model and the heuristic/systematic model.

Design an ad for a new product using the elaboration likelihood model and the heuristic/systematic model.

Homework Answers

Answer #1

Persuasion is a complicated thing. Everyone who talks uses persuasion at one time or another and we are all different, living in different situations, seeking different goals. No one simple thing can cover all the ways people use persuasion.

But one slightly complicated theory comes as close as anything I’ve seen. Like most good theories, it is built on simple parts, but when you combine these simple parts, the Elaboration Likelihood Model becomes almost as complicated as everyday life.

Assumptions of the Elaboration Likelihood Model

The ELM is quite simple and based on four assumptions about people and persuasion. First, let’s take the four assumptions in one fell swoop, then we’ll detail them.

1: There are two routes of thinking that a person may employ.

2: Situational and personality variables affect which route of thinking a person will employ.

3: Persuasion tools will have different effects depending upon the route of thinking employed.

4: Change achieved through the Central Route is more persistent over time, more resistant to change, and more predictive of behavior than change from the Peripheral Route.

Assumption 1: There are two routes of thinking that a person may employ.

One route is called the “Central Route ” and the other is called the “Peripheral Route.” The Central Route refers to a person who is carefully and effortfully thinking. The thought process is active, creative, and alert. The Peripheral Route, by contrast, is at the other extreme of thinking. Here the person is not really thinking very carefully and instead is skimming along the surface of ideas. They are thinking enough to be aware of the situation, but they are not thinking carefully enough to catch flaws, errors, and inconsistencies in the situation.

The big distinction between the two Routes is the amount of – hold on cause I can’t avoid this – elaboration activity. People on the Central Route engage in extensive elaborative processing while people on the Peripheral Route don’t. Stated another way, people on the Central Route think more thoughts and more relevant thougths about the situation than people on the Peripheral Route.

Let’s make two sharp points of exception right now.

1. Just because the conversation is short doesn’t mean it is Peripheral. “They all taste the same to me, give me the cheapest” is a short conversation, but it is central. Sometimes Central Route thinking is shorter because it cuts right to chase and finds the key element that determines the attitude.

2. Just because the conversation is long, doesn’t mean it is Central. If the thoughts are irrelevant to the situation, it doesn’t matter how many of them you generate. If our shopper is having a great and involved conversation that includes humming along to the music and remembering that time back in high school and wondering what happened to that fabulous blonde and that one date, wow, then, oops, walked past the spaghetti sauce, isn’t that on the list, hey, that red label looks good, I’ll bet it tastes great . . . you’ve got a long peripheral conversation.

Assumption 2: Situational and personality variables affect which route of thinking a person will employ.

People can flexibly move back and forth between the two Routes. Sometimes we are Central and other times we are Peripheral. (It is easy to confuse routes of thinking with intelligence as if smart people are central and dumb people are peripheral. While there is overlap, keep the concepts clean and separate. Smart people amble down the Peripheral Route all the time. Dumb people can focus their more limited resources and take the Central Route when needed.)

Assumption 3: Persuasion tools will have different effects depending upon the route of thinking employed.

When people are taking the Central Route, certain things will be important and useful to them. While reading that editorial on executing persuasion readers, the central thinker will be looking for facts, evidence, examples, reasoning, and logic. We call these things, “Arguments.”

By contrast, when people are taking the Peripheral Route, other things will be important. Since Arguments (facts, evidence, reasoning, etc.) require cognitive effort and energy, the peripheral thinker won’t notice or use them often. Instead, easier to process information will be employed. Things like the attractiveness, friendliness, or expertise of the source will be more useful to the peripheral thinker. We call these things, “Cues.” Thus, there are two persuasion tools, Arguments and Cues.

Assumption 4: Change achieved through the Central Route is more persistent over time, more resistant to change, and more predictive of behavior than change from the Peripheral Route.

When people are thinking centrally, if they do change, it is more likely to stick precisely because they thought about it more carefully, fully, and deeply. For peripheral thinkers, however, any change is likely to be rather short lived, simply because they did not really think that much. An enormous amount of research indicates that the more thinking, more elaboration activity, more long conversation in the head creates more change because it generates more connections in the web of your memory. Thus, even the most modern persuasion theory, the ELM, pivots in part on the oldest persuasion play, the Ding-Dong of Classical Conditioning, as elaborations get associated with topics and attitudes. By contrast, peripheral route folks are ambling along singing a song Cue by Cue forgetting what they just saw or heard as they move onto the next New Thing. Why wouldn’t we expect the Central Route to produce change that is more persistent, resistant, and predictive?

Are There Really Only Two Routes?

The assumptions of the dual process approach make it sound like we have a mental switch that moves us from one track to another. Are there really only two routes of thinking? Is there nothing in between? Most current research seems to say that there are only two routes with nothing in between. This simplifies the theory, but catch a key point here.

Please realize that the kind of persuasion we are talking about here is that immediate, here-and-now, dynamic, flow of life, it only happens once and it is happening right now, you only get one chance to make a first impression, kind of persuasion. Realize that life is made of up all of those moments and that we hold all of these events in our memories, saving them for use in later times. Thus, at this moment we are centrally processing an argument about voting for a candidate and then at that moment we are peripherally processing a cue about voting for the same person. In each situation we can clearly see the operation of only one route, central or peripheral. However, if we think about the total experience of the person in regards to that candidate, their beliefs and attitudes were created by the combination of these central and peripheral experiences. Thus, there are only two Routes, but we use them both for the same issue or topic and accumulate a position based on all the experiences.

Researchers have made a very useful bifurcation in the Central Route. Several studies and common sense thinking reveal that Central Route thinkers may be on two different sub-routes, either Objective or Biased. Objective route thinkers do all of that effortful elaboration activity, but it is driven by the quality of the Arguments. By contrast, Biased route thinkers engage in elaboration activity, but the thoughts are controlled by an existing bias.

Let’s pull on this distinction a bit. Objective thinkers are doing the best they can to determine the “truth” of the persuasive situation as closely as it is possible to determine the “truth.” Which candidate should you vote for in the next election? While the answer to that question is not as simple and unambiguous as “What does 2 + 2 equal?” it is possible to make voting selection a highly rational and empirical process. Objective thinkers do their best to find all of the relevant information under these uncertain conditions and if not find the “true” answer, at least find the “best” answer.

By contrast, the Biased thinker will not follow the data wherever it leads, but rather will tend to select Arguments and generate elaborations that are congenial to an existing bias. In our election example, if you were raised in a conservative household, chances are pretty good that you already hold favorable beliefs and attitudes towards candidates with a conservative orientation. When you think about the next election when you think about the candidates you will unconsciously tend to pick arguments that bias toward the conservative. For example, you’ll look for candidate voting records only on the conservative positions (foreign policy, free markets) and ignore more liberal positions (human rights, international cooperation). And, even though the Biased thinker is carefully and effortfully elaborating on arguments, the deck is a bit stacked in favor of an existing position.

Summary: There are two big Routes: Central and Peripheral. Within Central Route there are two sub routes: Objective and Biased. Both Objective and Biased processors are high WATT in contrast to Peripheral processors who are more top of the head, low WATT. However, Objective and Biased thinkers use their cognitive resources in very different ways. Objective processors try to follow the trail of Arguments wherever they lead while Biased processors try to make the trail lead to a predetermined position.

Review the Routes in Detail

While each element of the ELM is fairly simple, the model can get complex. Let’s track out each Route to see the whole thing in operation.

On the Central Route receivers are given an Elaboration Moderator that increases their Elaboration Likelihood which means they are high WATT. This combination of high willingness and ability leads them to look for Arguments, crucial pieces of information about the issue. As they find and consider Arguments, they generate Elaborations, unique cognitive responses, in that “long conversation” in the head. If these Elaborations are positive, they will generate positive attitude change (”This is good”) and if the Elaborations are negative, they will generate negative attitude change (”This is bad.”) In ELM parlance we say that Strong Arguments generate positive, favorable Elaborations while Weak Arguments generate negative, unfavorable Elaborations.

Now, if the WATTage generates Biased processing, receivers will still follow the same sequence of operations, but the Biasing treatment will also activate an existing schema, value, or belief that then guides processing of Arguments. Instead of trying to go where the Argument leads, Biased processors will cut and paste elaborations to make the Arguments fit with the Biased schema, value, belief, etc. This Bias is most obvious with Weak Arguments. Where the Objective processor would look at Weak Arguments and generate negative Elaborations, the Biased processor will tend to make more positive Elaborations. Consider fans of major league baseball. When considering the “correct” evaluation of steroid users, Objective processors will tend to call it like they see it. By contrast, Biased fans may minimize the problem (the Weak Argument) and more highly value other elements (the Strong Arguments). Both Objective and Biased processors are high WATT, generate Elaborations, and work the Central Route to change. Objective processors follow the Arguments; Biased processors make the Arguments fit a pre-existing belief, schema, or value.

Finally, on the Peripheral Route, receivers get Elaboration Moderators that reduce Elaboration Likelihood and produce low WATT processing. Since these folks lack sufficient motivation or ability, they will miss or avoid Arguments, but find Cues. If the Cue is positive, then positive attitude change will result, but if the Cue is negative, then negative attitude change will occur. Low WATT processors are thinking and they perceive all the elements in the persuasion situation. They just do not engage in that effortful Elaboration Activity, that long conversation in the head about Arguments.

Typically researchers measure elaboration activity with simple method called, “Thought Listing.” They expose people to a combination of ELM variables and afterwards provide a sheet of paper and simply instruct each person to write down all the thoughts that occurred to them during the experiment. The thoughts are then analyzed in a simple fashion. First, the written statements are “unitized,” that is, broken down into single ideas, so that if someone wrote one long sentence, it wouldn’t be just one thought, but rather each unique idea in the sentence is made into a single unit. Second, each unit is classified as either “relevant” or “irrelevant” to the issue. Third, each unit is classified as either positive (favorable), negative (unfavorable), or neutral. If you’ve done the experiment right, Central Route processors will write down a lot of relevant thoughts (and few irrelevant) and if they got Strong Arguments those relevant thoughts will be positive, but if they got Weak Arguments, the relevant thoughts will be negative. They will also generate a low number of irrelevant thoughts. Peripheral processors tend to show a somewhat random or haphazard pattern often times with equal number of relevant and irrelevant thoughts, and positive and negative thoughts.

Mapping the Routes

We can also visualize the operation of all these ELM variables, WATTage, Arguments, and Cues with the aid of charts. Let’s start off with an empty chart to get oriented. On the left hand line (the vertical axis, the up and down line, the ordinate) we will put scores for Attitude with a plus + sign for favorable and a minus – sign for unfavorable. On the bottom line (the horizontal axis, the left to right line, the abscissa) we’ll put a category variable like WATTage (low and high) or Argument Quality (weak or strong) or Cue (negative or positive). Stated another way, we put scores for the dependent variable, Attitude, on the up and down line, and values for the independent variable, WATTage for example, on the left to right line. You remember that independent variables cause dependent variables, right? Here’s an empty example with two levels for WATTage (low and high) and two levels for Arg (weak and strong).

In this experiment, there would be four groups for the four unique combinations of WATTage (low and high) and Argument (weak and strong). After exposure to the unique combination (e.g. high WATT getting strong Args), the person would indicate their Attitude. Now, we’ll fill in a chart with an idealized, theory prediction, something you almost never achieve in a real life experiment, but when you get close, you don’t care about grant applications, journal publications, tenure decisions or anything else: It Worked! Imagine we have four groups of people and each are randomly assigned to get either a high WATT or a low WATT Elaboration Moderator (like relevance) and then receive either strong or weak Arguments. Theory says it should look like this.


Note the fan shape of the chart lines. As you move from low to high WATT, people have more motivation and ability to process Arguments and hence they begin to react to the difference between strong and weak Arguments. Thus, at low WATT, the Attitude scores for both Argument groups are the same, because nobody has the WATTage to see the difference. But, at high WATT, the fan opens and people start scrutinizing Arguments, Elaborating on them, and moving their Attitudes with the direction of the thoughts. If you got strong Args, you have a favorable Attitude, but if you got weak Args, you have an unfavorable Attitude. This fan is the hallmark of Objective Processing.

Now, let’s demonstrate what happens with Biased Processing. We do the same experiment, but now make the Elaboration Moderator something that causes a strong guiding belief, schema, value, etc. to activate either “low” or “high.” We then provide the same Arguments we just used, but now they are being considered by Biased processors. Note what happens to the fan.

See how the fan shifts up so that the Attitude change increases more for the Biased processor compared to the Objective processor. (We could also make the fan shift down the same way if the topic or issue was counter-attitudinal or against the initial belief.) Here, the Biased processor is making the Arguments “better” than an Objective processor would evaluate them. Realize that the Args are the same things in each example. What changes is how the person looking at the Arguments is using Them.

We can chart the effect with Cues instead of Args. Imagine that we have the same Objective Elaboration Moderator as in our earlier example, but now instead of giving Args, we only provide Cues. Here’s the idealized theory prediction.

Once, again, we get that lovely fan effect, but now it moves in a different direction. The fan opens and closes with the operation of the Cues. At high WATT, receivers want Args, but only get Cues which do not feed the hungry jaws of a high WATT processor – who cares if you see a pretty person or an ugly person when you are high WATT. But, with low WATT processors, they amble along the Peripheral Route, following Cues where they go.

Now, let’s do the full monty here. Let’s run an experiment with low and high WATTage, weak and strong Args, and negative and positive Cues. That will require a chart with two panels. Again, consider a blank chart just to get oriented.

The panel on the left side is for all the low WATT participants and the panel on the right is for all the high WATT participants. On the left to right lines, the abscissa, for each panel we’ll put values for each Cue and within the body of each panel we’ll put values for each Argument. The dependent variable score of Attitude stays on the up and down line, the ordinate. Got it? All we are doing is reproducing the same panel twice, one on the left hand side, the other on the right hand side. Now, let’s do an experiment with all 8 groups (2 WATTage X 2 Arg X 2 Cue) randomly assigned to each unique combination. Here’s the theory prediction.

The fan effect goes away in this display and we’re left we two sets of parallel lines. On the left hand, low WATT panel, the diagonal parallel lines show the effect of Cues while the right hand, high WATT panel shows separated parallels to indicate the effect of Args. Theory says, low WATT processors cannot handle Args since they lack either willingness or ability, so they follow Cues. By contrast, high WATT processors will ignore Cues, and instead consider Args.

These chart provide a different and powerful way for understanding the ELM. You can now visualize the ideal outcomes for Objective Processing of Arguments, Biased Processing of Arguments, Objective Processing of Cues, and finally, the full Dual Process of Arguments and Cues. Now, also realize that these patterns of fans and parallel lines depend upon how you locate WATTage, Args, and Cues in the chart. Things look very different if you put WATTage in a different part of the chart because the lines will create different patterns. To reproduce this, always remember to put WATTage on the horizon line, the left to right line. Otherwise, it will look like a trick question on a comprehensive exam!

How Do You Shift Routes?

This is a million dollar question.

We realize that Central thinkers want Arguments and Peripheral thinkers want Cues. We also know that Central thinkers, if they are influenced, will show changes that are more persistent, resistant, and predictive. How do we get people to take the Central Route ?

First of all, a lot of research and simple common sense indicates that most people most of the time are in the Peripheral Route. They are sometimes called, “cognitive misers.” Less politely, it means that people are lazy thinkers who do not want to expend the energy needed to think carefully and effortfully about something.

If you don’t believe me and think that people are instead usually Central thinkers, I have a task for you. Stop reading this chapter right now and go over to the TV. Turn it on and watch only the commercials for a while. For the overwhelming majority of ads, how much thinking do you have to do? That’s right, not much. Now, if people were usually in that Central Route, advertisers would not show the kind of commercials we see. Ads are long on Cues and short on Arguments.

People think enough to meet the minimum demands of the situation. That is the status quo and it means we spend most of our time on the Peripheral Route. But as a practical persuader, we have a different agenda. We want our targets to change and we want that change to last. That means Central thinking, which returns us to the million dollar question.

Quite surprisingly, there are many ways to get central thinking (and if you want to read more about it do a search on “elaboration likelihood”). The best way to get a handle on these mental switches is to think about two big categories: Willingness and Ability.

If you want to increase the likelihood of Central processing you must make sure that your target is Willing to do this kind of cognitive work. Central processing by definition takes more concentration and effort than usual. When you want people to work harder, you must motivate and enable them. If you read the literature on this, you will find a huge list of variables that affect WATTage: comprehension, distraction, repetition, posture, relevance, responsibility, need for cognition, number of sources, forewarning, bogus feedback, audience reaction, dissonance, schema, induced hypocrisy, just to name fourteen off the top of my head. We can’t look at each, so I’ll just hit you with two obvious ones.

The best example of a factor that motivates is relevance. When people believe the situation is personally important to them, they are much more likely to think centrally about it. If the situation holds little relevance, they will stay in the Peripheral Route. Thus, you must demonstrate how the issue is meaningful and relevant to your targets if you want them to be central thinkers. In other words, your targets must be motivated to think.

The second factor you need to consider is Ability. When people have the ability to work harder, it is more likely they will do so. What makes people better able to think harder? Hey, if you’re selling your product to a mass audience that includes Hispanics, you might consider writing some of your ads in Spanish. If you are selling a high tech product to a group of low tech buyers, you might translate your PowerPoint presentation from Scientese into Just Folks.

Basically, what we are talking about here is comprehension. Can your target understand what you are talking about? Now, don’t give me any lip about this and tell me sad stories about stupid customers, clients, suppliers, friends, or family. You are trying to persuade them. If they don’t change, you did something wrong. When a source presents information that is complex, dense, abstruse, recondite, esoteric (I’m running out of entries in my thesaurus!), in other words when receivers have to work too hard to understand, they will not centrally process the information. Instead they will drop back into a Peripheral Route. In essence, you must make sure that your targets have the ability to think about the issue.

Keep your eye on the Main Point here: All of these variables function (function, function, function) as switches for WATTage. They answer the question, How do I turn their dimmer switch? Anything that moves the dial from low WATT to high WATT and vice versa, is an Elaboration Moderator.

“Elaboration” Sidebar

Realize that we’ve used the term, “elaboration,” in several different senses and each means something different, unique, and critical. Consider this list.

Elaboration – a unique thought, a cognitive response that is relevant. Receivers generate Elaborations.

Elaboration Moderator – a variable that affects Elaboration Likelihood. Sources manipulate or monitor Moderators.

Elaboration Likelihood – willingness and ability to think (WATTage). Receivers have the dimmer switch.

Elaboration Activity – the “long conversation” in the head with lots of issue relevant thoughts (Elaborations). Receivers converse in their heads.

Obviously, each term shares a key element – that user generated relevant thought. Elaboration is the key to understanding persuasion. No wonder they call it the Elaboration Likelihood Model, right?

What’s The List Of Arguments And Cues?

Once we have established the route, we have to provide the correct influence agent. If we get the right match between route and agent, we are successful. If we get the wrong match, we have a problem.

Central thinkers want arguments. It should be easy to produce lists of arguments and away we go. But, hold on a minute and think about this.

Your teenage son needs a new pair of sneakers. Assume that both you and your son are going to be central thinkers as you decide which sneakers to buy. Okay, you’re both central thinkers, so that means you both want arguments. From your point of view, what are the arguments you consider as you choose between the various sneakers?

How much do they cost?
How long will they last?
Is the store nearby?

Now, consider the list of arguments from your teenager’s perspective.

Who endorses them?
Do all the other guys wear them?
Would that great looking girl go out with me if I had ‘em?

You see the problem. Arguments depend upon the receiver. Thus, to develop a list of arguments for any given persuasion situation requires some careful thought on the part of the persuasion source. In essence a persuasion source asks this question: What is of central importance to the receiver?

If you can figure out the answers to this question and the receiver is in the Central Route, then you will be effective as a persuader. You will also generate persistent, resistant, and predictive attitude change.

This is an important point and I want to give you an example to illustrate the “relative” meaning of Arguments. The example concerns teenagers and smoking. In the past, persuasion sources (parents, teachers, the federal government) have tried to prevent teenage smoking with arguments based on health (”smoking causes cancer”). And despite the best efforts of all concerned, teens continued to smoke. Why? The health Argument lacks central importance to a teenager. Teenagers still embrace the myth of immortality and they know they will live forever, maybe even to forty. Threats about cancer and death are empty.

New approaches use different arguments and have shown better results. The new Arguments are based on social factors (”you smell bad if you smoke,” “no one wants to kiss somebody with cigarette breath”) and more lately on control factors (“greedy tobacco companies are manipulating you”). Peer acceptance and independence are of central importance to teens. These arguments appear to be more powerful to teenagers and hence produce the kind of change we prefer.

The main point is this: There is no cookbook list of Arguments because argument quality depends upon the receiver. To produce good arguments, you must understand your receivers and be able to think the way they do.

Watch the beer commercials on TV for the answer. Young, attractive women wearing skimpy bikinis are among the dominant images on these ads. No reasonable person would ever claim that these young women are arguments for the beer. (Unless the receiver really believes that the girl comes with the case.)

Well, then maybe those girls are simply used to get the receiver’s attention and make them central thinkers about the beer ad. If you believe that, I’ve got a bridge you might want to buy. Half naked women do not encourage central thinking about beer.

The only thing we are left with is a cue. People (usually men) watch the beer ad and see the attractive girls. The men like this in a way that requires little thinking on their part. And they simply associate that good feeling with the beer and voila, influence without thought.

But does it work?

Bet the ranch on it. The effects of attractiveness on the peripheral processor are very powerful.

Here are more quick examples.

Ever been to a Tupperware party and ended up buying something you don’t really need or want simply because you see you other friends buying things?

Have you ever seen a good kid do something really stupid just because the peer group expects it?

There are many other cues. (If you can’t wait, go to CLARCCS Cues, Does It Come In Red?). There are a lot of persuasion Cues and we will discuss them throughout the rest of this book. In fact, Cues are so important I offer this rule:

When in Doubt, Take the Peripheral Route.

Or, stated Yet Another Way.

What To Do? Use a Cue!

Unless, of course, you’ve got a High WATT thinker and Strong Arguments.

Practical Implications

The ELM has three important implications for us. While the model sounds abstract, it does apply to our everyday life in practical ways. Consider these ideas.

1. Monitor and control the mental state.

This is a major point. Persuaders who are adept at reading their targets are much more likely to be effective persuaders. But how can you tell which mental state your target is in? There are two primary areas to look at.

First, observe the nonverbal behaviors of your targets. Generally speaking, if you observe behaviors that indicate attentiveness, alertness, and thoughtfulness, you can begin to assume those targets are in the Central Route. Use this acronym: SOLER.

Squarely face
Open posture
Lean in
Eye contact
Relaxation

When receivers are squarely looking at you with good eye contact with relaxed and open body posture while leaning toward you, it’s a good indication they are hot SOLER and high WATT. As they change these nonverbal indicators the SOLER goes cold and WATTage goes low.

Second, simply ask questions. Get your targets to respond. Then judge the quality of the responses. Do they sound thoughtful and reasonable? Or instead, do they ask you to repeat the question or give answers that are off the wall?

When people are on the Central Route they look and act differently than when they are in the Peripheral Route. Learn to get a sense of this by observing your targets. It will make you a nicer person, too.

2. Match the right influence agent (Arguments or Cues) with the correct mental state (high or low WATTage).

You don’t need an umbrella on a sunny day and peripheral processors will not heed arguments. You have to identify correctly what the receivers mental state is and then provide either arguments or cues.

In most situations most of the time with most people, they ambling along the Peripheral Route. They are not focused like a laser beam on every element of their environment carefully considering the pros and cons, the costs and benefits, the profits and losses of those elements (“Yes, it tastes great, but is it really filling!”). Again, if you don’t believe me, just look at any kind of advertising. Really look around a mall or a busy shopping district. Look at TV or Internet ads. Overwhelmingly, you see Cues. It’s not even close.

This is not to say that we should not use Arguments in most ordinary circumstances. As we have already developed, when people systematically think about persuasion arguments, the influence will last longer, be more resistant to change, and motivate behavior. We want that kind of change. But first you must make sure that your targets are willing and able to do the needed thinking. If you cannot assure yourself that your receivers are in such a frame of mind, it is useless and frustrating to try and influence with arguments.

3. Develop Arguments from the point of view of the receiver.

Okay, we need the obligatory example of some bonehead play. How about something from long ago and far away?

During the mid-1980s (shortly after hamburgers were invented), Burger King spent millions of dollars on a major advertising campaign. The purpose of this campaign was not merely to sell a few more burgers, but to challenge McDonald’s for leadership in the very competitive fast food market. Burger King did a lot of careful planning, a lot of quiet pretesting, and then unleashed its ad attack.

The campaign was built around a character named Herb. Herb was a balding, thin fellow, who wore glasses, black pants that were too short, and white socks. Herb was supposed to be a whimsical sort of Everyman that we could all identify with.  Here’s a YouTube of an early Herb ad.

Know the answer?
Your Answer:

Post as a guest

Your Name:

What's your source?

Earn Coins

Coins can be redeemed for fabulous gifts.

Not the answer you're looking for?
Ask your own homework help question
Similar Questions
1. Find one ad that uses a central route processing strategy. Describe how you know they...
1. Find one ad that uses a central route processing strategy. Describe how you know they are using central route processing and why it is a good example of a central route message. 2. Find another ad, preferably for a similar product ,that uses a peripheral route processing strategy. Describe how they are using the strategy and why it is a good example. 3. Draw a conclusion: Based on what you know about the Elaboration Likelihood Model - which processing...
Describe the Elaboration Likelihood Model. How do the central and peripheral cues in advertisements influence consumers’...
Describe the Elaboration Likelihood Model. How do the central and peripheral cues in advertisements influence consumers’ attitude towards ads and influence persuasion?
The Aggregate Demand – Aggregate Supply model (AD/AS) is a guide for policymaking. Explain, using an...
The Aggregate Demand – Aggregate Supply model (AD/AS) is a guide for policymaking. Explain, using an AD/AS model, the effect on the aggregate demand (AD) of expansionary monetary and fiscal policy. First draw the diagram and then explain the impact on aggregate demand (AD).
Using the IS-LM model combined with the AD-AS model, show and explain the effects in the...
Using the IS-LM model combined with the AD-AS model, show and explain the effects in the SR and LR from a decrease in the money supply - assuming nothing else exogenous changes. Be certain to explain how and why variables change and include graphs.
how the business model would affect the design of a new AIS. how the business model...
how the business model would affect the design of a new AIS. how the business model would affect the design of a new AIS.
Using the AD-AS model, an economy is at its long term equilibrium, growing at 1%. Government...
Using the AD-AS model, an economy is at its long term equilibrium, growing at 1%. Government decides to reduce taxes and increase government spending. What changes will be seen in the AD-AS model, how will the graph look, what effect does it have in economy?
how the business model would affect the design of a new AIS.
how the business model would affect the design of a new AIS.
Explain, using a carefully labeled graph, how the economy adjusts under the AS/AD model that we...
Explain, using a carefully labeled graph, how the economy adjusts under the AS/AD model that we covered in class when there is a cost-push shock.
Using the AD-AS model, show and explain how long-run real output (potential GDP) be affected by...
Using the AD-AS model, show and explain how long-run real output (potential GDP) be affected by a financial crisis.
Using the aggregate demand and aggregate supply model, show and explain what shifts in AD and...
Using the aggregate demand and aggregate supply model, show and explain what shifts in AD and AS resulted in the changes in prices and output that occurred during World War II in the U.S. The oil crisis began in October 1973 when the members of the Organization of Arab Petroleum Exporting Countries proclaimed an oil embargo. The embargo was targeted at nations perceived as supporting Israel during the Yom Kippur War. Using AD-AS graphs, show and explain the effect of...